Prosopagnosia


Definition/Introduction

Prosopagnosia is defined as the inability to recognize known and new faces. It is also known as facial/visual agnosia.[1] Bodamer first used the word prosopagnosia in 1947 in a landmark paper that described the cases of two patients with face recognition deficits.[2] The word comes from Greek prosopon, meaning face and agnosia, meaning lack of knowledge. Normally, an individual can recognize and remember 5000+ faces throughout their lifetime.[3]

Issues of Concern

There are varying degrees of impairment in prosopagnosia, including:

  • The inability to recognize
  • Discriminate
  • Identify different or own faces
  • Discern differences between faces and surrounding objects.

To compensate for their deficit, patients use their other senses and cues such as voice, shapes, and anomalous contours of the face. These compensatory mechanisms, at times, are not enough to recognize familiar faces. This impairment causes a psychological and social impact leading to functional impairment, unemployment, social isolation, depression, anxiety, and other mood disorders.[4] Peculiarly, patients complain of having trouble following television shows and movies because they cannot visually keep track of characters.

Clinical Significance

Prosopagnosia can be acquired or hereditary. Acquired cases can resutl from ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke, traumatic brain injury, certain neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric illnesses (Alzheimer disease, depression, and schizophrenia). Hereditary or development etiologies are a hot area of research, given that they are more common than acquired etiologies. The prevalence can approach 2.5% of the population.[5] The mechanism of inheritance is not totally clear but thought to be autosomal dominant.[5] Patients with juvenile prosopagnosia cannot recognize faces throughout their life, and a strong family history is usually present. It can be present in children with developmental disorders, including autism and Asperger syndrome. The impairment affects social development. Difficulties include lack of fear of strangers, intense separation anxiety, behavioral issues, and refusal to perform tasks that required face recognition.

Variants of prosopagnosia include an apperceptive variant (deficits in facial structure perception), and amnestic or associative variant (unable to remember faces even though they can perceive them; the perceptual information can not access facial memories because of a disconnection or loss of them).[6]

Localization/Pathophysiology

The pathophysiology is still not completely understood and is a prominent area of research. Damage or developmental anomaly in the right or bilateral fusiform-lingual gyrus, with neuron pathways that control facial perception and memory, are thought to be involved. Imaging research shows that deficits in the temporal cortex and amygdala can also be involved. Individuals with fusiform lesions are more likely to have an apperceptive variant, while those with anterior temporal lobe lesions have the amnesia variant. Development prosopagnosia appears to be a result of either a disconnection between the anterior and posterior face networks or reduced functional activation of regions responsible for facial recognition and identification. Advanced imaging studies support these concepts. The hypothesis is that dysfunctional neural migration during development may be the mechanism behind juvenile prosopagnosia.[7]

Neuropsychological Diagnostic Tests

Diagnostic tests can divide into three main types. First, perception tests that can accurately evaluate the patient's ability to discriminate between different visual facial stimuli. Second, recognition tests that can specifically assess short and long term recognition patterns of faces. Third, facial identification tests that involve other auditory or tactile cues (such as naming) to help identify facial stimuli. Examples for face perception tests are the Cambridge Face Perception Test, Glasgow Face Matching Test, Benton Facial Recognition, and the Caledonian Face Test.[8][9][10]

Facial memories are testable through imagery.[11] Other tools include self-report questionnaires, including the Cambridge Face Memory Questionnaire, Kennerknecht 15-item questionnaire, and the 20-item Prosopagnosia Index. These instruments of research tend to have low sensitivity and specificity for diagnosing prosopagnosia; therefore, objective measures should be pursued.

Imaging

Advanced imaging is a research tool that uses properties such as cortical thickness, functional activation, and connectivity to study the networks involved in different types of prosopagnosia. Studies using positron-emission tomography scan and functional magnetic resonance imaging show that facial recognition networks are located in multiple brain regions, including the anterior temporal lobe, prefrontal cortex, inferior and middle temporal cortex, the hippocampus, the amygdala, and most importantly, and the fusiform face area or occipitotemporal gyrus. Activation of the fusiform face area in the non-dominant hemisphere appears to be involved in processing, while activation in the dominant hemisphere is associated with analytic processing. The perirhinal cortex in the medial temporal lobe is also involved in familiarity-based recognition. The different brain regions work synergistically to encode, store, and retrieve memories related to face recognition.

Nursing, Allied Health, and Interprofessional Team Interventions

At this time, there is no evidence-based study that guides the creation of an interdisciplinary team for prosopagnosia. However, a neurologist, neuropsychologist, social worker, mental health counselor, psychiatrist, and geneticist should be consulted to provide the best care and quality of life for a patient with familial and acquired prosopagnosia. [Level ]


Details

Updated:

5/1/2023 6:29:43 PM

References


[1]

Corrow SL, Dalrymple KA, Barton JJ. Prosopagnosia: current perspectives. Eye and brain. 2016:8():165-175. doi: 10.2147/EB.S92838. Epub 2016 Sep 26     [PubMed PMID: 28539812]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[2]

BODAMER J. [Prosop's agnosia; the agnosia of cognition]. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, vereinigt mit Zeitschrift fur die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie. 1947:118(1-2):6-53     [PubMed PMID: 18901747]


[3]

Jenkins R, Dowsett AJ, Burton AM. How many faces do people know? Proceedings. Biological sciences. 2018 Oct 10:285(1888):. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2018.1319. Epub 2018 Oct 10     [PubMed PMID: 30305434]


[4]

Yardley L, McDermott L, Pisarski S, Duchaine B, Nakayama K. Psychosocial consequences of developmental prosopagnosia: a problem of recognition. Journal of psychosomatic research. 2008 Nov:65(5):445-51. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.03.013. Epub 2008 Aug 15     [PubMed PMID: 18940375]


[5]

Kennerknecht I, Plümpe N, Edwards S, Raman R. Hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA): the first report outside the Caucasian population. Journal of human genetics. 2007:52(3):230-236. doi: 10.1007/s10038-006-0101-6. Epub 2006 Dec 22     [PubMed PMID: 17186317]


[6]

Davies-Thompson J, Pancaroglu R, Barton J. Acquired prosopagnosia: structural basis and processing impairments. Frontiers in bioscience (Elite edition). 2014 Jan 1:6(1):159-74     [PubMed PMID: 24389150]


[7]

Albonico A, Barton J. Progress in perceptual research: the case of prosopagnosia. F1000Research. 2019:8():. pii: F1000 Faculty Rev-765. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.18492.1. Epub 2019 May 31     [PubMed PMID: 31231507]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[8]

Duchaine B, Yovel G, Nakayama K. No global processing deficit in the Navon task in 14 developmental prosopagnosics. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2007 Jun:2(2):104-13. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm003. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 18985129]


[9]

Burton AM, White D, McNeill A. The Glasgow Face Matching Test. Behavior research methods. 2010 Feb:42(1):286-91. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.286. Epub     [PubMed PMID: 20160307]


[10]

Logan AJ, Wilkinson F, Wilson HR, Gordon GE, Loffler G. The Caledonian face test: A new test of face discrimination. Vision research. 2016 Feb:119():29-41. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.11.003. Epub 2016 Jan 19     [PubMed PMID: 26607479]


[11]

Barton JJ, Cherkasova M. Face imagery and its relation to perception and covert recognition in prosopagnosia. Neurology. 2003 Jul 22:61(2):220-5     [PubMed PMID: 12874402]