Agraphia

Earn CME/CE in your profession:


Continuing Education Activity

Agraphia is an impairment or loss of a previous ability to write. Agraphia can occur in isolation, although it often occurs concurrently with other neurologic deficits such as alexia, apraxia, or hemispatial neglect. Agraphia is an impairment or loss of a previous ability to write and can result from a number of etiologies. When agraphia results from disruptions to neurologic processes directly involved in language, this results in central agraphia, and when agraphia results from the motor output of writing, this results in peripheral agraphia. Proper characterization of agraphia is essential in determining the right treatment. This activity reviews the evaluation and treatment of agraphia and highlights the role of the interprofessional team in managing patients with this condition.

Objectives:

  • Describe the pathophysiology of agraphia.
  • Explain the common physical exam findings associated with agraphia.
  • Summarize the management considerations for patients with agraphia.
  • Outline the importance of collaboration and communication amongst the interprofessional team to enhance care delivery for patients affected by agraphia.

Introduction

Agraphia is an impairment or loss of a previous ability to write. Agraphia can occur in isolation, although it often occurs concurrently with other neurologic deficits such as alexia, apraxia, or hemispatial neglect. Clinically, agraphia can be divided into “central” agraphia (also called “linguistic” or “aphasic“ agraphia) and “peripheral” agraphia (also called “nonlinguistic” or “nonaphasic” agraphia).[1]

To perform the act of writing, an individual produces a series of “graphemes” to communicate meaningful information. In the English language, graphemes consist of letters of the Latin alphabet. To write properly, one must first have knowledge of the letters themselves, and then one must know how to organize letters to form proper words and grammatically correct sentences. Lesions disrupting these processes result in central agraphia. The individual must then know the set of coordinated movements to draw out letters (praxis) correctly, the ability to “mentally queue up” a sequence of letters to make an entire word (motor programming), the visuospatial ability to guide a writing implement on a writing surface, and finally, the motor system to carry out these tasks.[1][2] Impairment of these latter steps involved in the motor planning or motor action of writing leads to peripheral agraphia. Note that “peripheral” agraphia can localize to the central nervous system when it does not directly involve linguistic centers, such as in the case of motor agraphia due to a motor cortex lesion leading to the paresis of the writing limb.

Clinical neurologic and neuropsychological classification schemas exist to categorize the agraphia, and a clinical neurologic classification is used in this review. “Pure agraphia” is a term used to refer to an isolated impairment of writing without an associated relevant impairment in either language ability or praxis. While certain authors equate “pure agraphia” with apraxic agraphia, there is an effort by other authors to distinguish between “pure linguistic (or aphasic) agraphia” and “pure apraxic agraphia.”[3] This review agrees with making this distinction on both a clinical and anatomic basis, and the evidence for supporting this distinction is discussed throughout this article.It must also be noted that the term “aphasia” is used inconsistently in literature. Aphasia most commonly refers to an acquired impairment of just spoken language, but it is also frequently used to encompass both written and spoken language impairments.[2][4][5] 

This article uses the term “aphasia” to refer to an isolated impairment of spoken language. The peripheral agraphias can be viewed as analogous to “dysarthrias” of written language in the sense that “dysarthria” denotes an impairment in the motor output of verbal language. Furthermore, in central agraphia, it is possible for there to be a dissociation between written and spoken language ability; in some cases, there is even a difference in the type of aphasia manifested in the same patient, such as verbal nonfluent aphasia accompanied by written fluent aphasia.[6][7] The qualifier term “linguistic” is used in this review to encompass issues of both written and spoken language.

Agraphia is distinguished from illiteracy, in which the ability to write is never obtained. The term “dysgraphia” is used most commonly to denote handwriting impairment as part of a primary learning disability, but it is also sometimes used to denote an incomplete acquired writing impairment and, in this case, is synonymous with agraphia.[8] “Paragraphia” refers to a specific writing error, and they are analogous to the paraphasic errors of speech. The impairment of typing on a keyboard--dystypia, or dystextia in the case of mobile phone use--often accompanies agraphia, but cases of isolated dystypia or dystextia suggest these may represent distinct impairments.[9]

  • Central (Linguistic) Agraphia
    • Agraphia with Nonfluent Aphasia
      • Agraphia, with nonfluent aphasia, typically reflects features of Broca’s aphasia, also referred to as nonfluent or motor aphasia. Letter and word output are lower than expected, with spelling errors due to letter omission. There may also be agrammatism marked by a paucity of prepositions and other grammatical elements, and calligraphy is poor. Dissociation between written language and verbal language abilities may be present, and written language may be inferior to verbal language.
    • Agraphia with Fluent Aphasia[10]
      • Agraphia, with fluent aphasia, typically reflects the impairments in Wernicke’s aphasia, also referred to as fluent or sensory aphasia. Patients characteristically produce a normal quantity of words with normal calligraphy, but the content is nonsensical. There may be neologisms (“jargonagraphia”), as well as the nonsensical use or placement of proper words. Grammatical elements may be overused in relation to nouns. Written language ability may be superior to verbal language—sometimes markedly so.[11]
    • Agraphia with Conduction Aphasia[12]
      • Agraphia with conduction aphasia has been rarely described. The hallmark of conduction aphasia is a characteristic impairment of repetition known as the “conduit d’approche”: the patient will incorrectly repeat a word but sequentially iterate through phonologically proximate variations until the correct pronunciation is reached. Written analogs of the “conduit d’approche” have been described in patients with conduction aphasia.
    • Alexia with Agraphia[13]
      • Alexia with agraphia refers to an impairment of both writing and reading ability, which can occur with or without aphasia. Patterns of written language impairment may follow those seen in the above categories.
  • Pure Linguistic Agraphia vs. Pure Apraxic Agraphia
    • An isolated impairment of writing that is not accompanied by any other language impairment or impairment in praxis can be considered “pure” agraphia. However, effort should be made to distinguish isolated agraphia due to a linguistic deficit from isolated agraphia due to writing apraxia.
    • Pure Linguistic Agraphia[14][15][16][17][18]
      • Pure linguistic agraphia is present when an isolated impairment in written language occurs as a result of a disruption to central linguistic processes involved in writing. Verbal language and reading abilities are intact. There may be semantic and/or orthographic errors in writing. Calligraphy quality is often normal. Case-based evidence suggests that pure linguistic agraphia may be further subdivided into phonological and lexical subtypes.[11][19] In phonological agraphia, there is an inability to spell pronounceable non-words and a retained ability to spell irregular words, which are exception words where spelling is dissociated from pronunciation--compare “steak” with “beak.” The reverse is found in lexical agraphia. Dyscravia is a more recently described disorder of phoneme-to-grapheme conversion in which written words are misspelled using proximate sounds, such as substituting a “t” for a “d”; dyscravia classifies as a subtype of phonological agraphia.[20]
    • Pure Apraxic Agraphia[17][21][22][23][24][25]
      • Pure apraxic agraphia is task-specific apraxia.[26] As with pure linguistic agraphia, in pure apraxic agraphia, there is an isolated impairment in written language in the presence of intact verbal language and reading ability. However, the writing impairment here stems from disruption to processes involved in the motor output of writing downstream of linguistic processes. As such, impaired calligraphy is often considered a hallmark of apraxic agraphia. There should be no other manifestation of ideomotor or ideational apraxia for apraxic agraphia to be considered “pure.” In some cases of pure apraxic agraphia, findings may be as subtle as a selective impairment in font, such as cursive script (“allographic agraphia”) or case (such as an isolated inability to write in upper case).[27][28]
    • Distinguishing between pure linguistic and pure apraxic agraphia
      • The distinction between pure linguistic apraxia and pure apraxic agraphia has been considered a significant diagnostic challenge. Certain techniques may help distinguish these two entities:[3]
      • Calligraphy (writing quality): Calligraphy is often impaired in pure apraxic agraphia. As such, several authors have suggested that the presence of intact calligraphy points to pure linguistic agraphia. However, it remains unclear if one can have an isolated impairment of calligraphy involving linguistic processes affected in central agraphia with nonfluent aphasia.
      • Spelling: Oral spelling, considered a non-motor modality of “writing,” may be affected in linguistic agraphia but intact in apraxic agraphia.
      • Copying: Writing that improves with copying written text may be useful in identifying pure linguistic agraphia on the grounds that the writing praxis is intact.
      • Non-motor writing: Besides oral spelling, other non-motor modalities to “write,” such as keyboard typing and mobile phone texting, may be preserved in pure apraxic agraphia and impaired in pure linguistic agraphia.
      • Pseudowords and irregular words: The writing of pseudowords and irregular words is used in the clinical neuropsychologic assessment of writing to localize disruptions in “phonological” and “lexical” processes, respectively.[11][29][30] Such techniques may help identify pure linguistic agraphia if writing impairments are limited to these specific linguistic features.
  • Peripheral (Nonlinguistic or Nonaphasic) Agraphia
    • Apraxic Agraphia[1][16]
      • In apraxic agraphia, there is an impairment in the motor planning of writing. Apraxic agraphia can occur with or without other forms of apraxia; when isolated, it can be designated as pure apraxic agraphia. Apraxic agraphia is most commonly a form of ideomotor apraxia, in which case the individual has the desire to write but is unable to do so. However, ideational agraphia--in which the knowledge of the concept of writing itself is lost--has also been described.[31]
      • Motor Agraphia
        • Motor agraphia is broad and occurs due to the disruption of the motor system downstream of praxis, anywhere from the motor cortex to the peripheral nerve and muscle. Paretic agraphia is a result of paresis regardless of localization. Micrographia is hypokinetic motor agraphia most commonly associated with Parkinsonism and attributed to bradykinesia; it is marked by a progressive decrement in handwriting size. Hyperkinetic agraphia can result from a number of movement disorders, such as tremors or chorea. The writer’s cramp is task-specific focal dystonia of the forearm and hand muscles activated by writing. A writer’s cramp is considered simple when isolated to writing and complex (or dystonic) when it carries over into other non-writing activities.[32]
      • Reiterative Agraphia
        • The reiterative agraphia includes features such as perseveration, echographia (rewriting of phrases produced by the examiner), or paligraphia (rewriting of the same phrase produced by the patient).
    • Visuospatial Agraphia[33]
      • Visuospatial agraphia is writing impairment due to errors of orientation to the writing instrument and/or writing surface. One common cause of visuospatial agraphia is hemispatial neglect, where one is unaware of half of the page--most frequently, this manifests with a blank left-hand side of a page. Other features suggesting visuospatial agraphia include errors of word spacing or word grouping and specific letter-writing errors such as incorrectly repeating strokes or even repeatedly writing the same letter multiple times. Letters or words may be superimposed. In these cases, writing errors are attributed to constructional apraxia. Note that the term “constructional apraxia” is a visuospatial disorder and does not refer to true apraxia (i.e., an error of a skilled motor task) and is not to be confused with apraxic agraphia.[34] Visuospatial agraphia can also be due to optic ataxia, in which there are errors of visual depth perception or oculomotor deficits.[35]
    • Functional (Psychogenic) Agraphia
      • Functional agraphia can also occur as a manifestation of conversion syndrome. As with other functional neurologic disorders, framing functional agraphia within the model of traditional neuroanatomic localization can be challenging.

Etiology

Any lesion can cause central or linguistic agraphia to the cortical language centers of the brain or to any of their associated subcortical structures. This is classically studied in stroke, which remains the most common cause of language impairment in general.[36] Virtually any lesion affecting these areas, including trauma, tumors, and infections, can result in central agraphia. Agraphia can present as a symptom of chemotoxicity, transient headache, and neurological deficits with cerebrospinal fluid lymphocytosis (HaNDL) or COVID-19 infection.[37][38][39]

Neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer disease or frontotemporal dementia can also lead to central agraphia, in which case the onset is gradual with progressive worsening. Primary progressive aphasia and its subtypes represent clinical neurodegenerative syndromes, which are characterized predominantly by slowly worsening language impairment; notably, Alzheimer disease and frontotemporal degeneration are implicated in the majority of these cases.[40][41] Agraphia is also considered the most common finding in delirium. However, delirium is nonspecific, and the etiologies of delirium are extremely varied.[1]

As with central agraphia, peripheral agraphia can be caused by a myriad of lesions anywhere from the cortex to the peripheral nerve and muscle. However, in peripheral agraphia, these lesions disrupt the motor planning or motor output of writing.

Epidemiology

Data on the incidence of acquired neurologic language impairment, including agraphia, is generally limited. Stroke is considered the most common cause of acquired language impairment.[1] One study in Switzerland found the annual incidence of language impairment due to a first-ever ischemic stroke to be 47 in 100,000 total inhabitants; of these, 30% of stroke patients developed aphasia. This study included patients with agraphia, although the rate of agraphia is not specified.[36][42]

Pathophysiology

The pathophysiology of neurologic language impairment, including aphasia and agraphia, remains incompletely understood. Generally, aphasia is better studied than agraphia and serves as the most common model for neurologic language processing. However, several reports have observed a dissociation between written language and verbal language ability, which suggests locations in the brain dedicated to writing.[7][10] With this said, several locations in the brain involved in language processing have been identified.

Classical localization methods based on vascular lesions originally identified two primary linguistic centers of the brain: these are Broca’s area in the dominant inferior frontal gyrus and Wernicke’s area in the dominant superior temporal gyrus. These areas are respectively supplied by the superior and inferior divisions of the middle cerebral artery. The left hemisphere is the dominant hemisphere in over 95% of right-handed and over 70% of left-handed individuals.[43] Lesions to Broca’s area typically lead to nonfluent language impairment; Broca’s area has been shown to be involved with linguistic functions, including fluency, phonological processing, grammar processing, and semantic retrieval.[44][45] 

Lesions to the inferior division of the middle cerebral artery are commonly attributed to “fluent” language impairment, characterized by nonsensical speech and impaired comprehension. It was initially believed and remains commonly taught that Wernicke’s area involves word recognition and meaning. However, while more recent data reveals that Wernicke’s area is indeed involved with phonological language production, it may not be crucial for language recognition. Rather, the posterior cortical language area responsible for word recognition and meaning- for which lesions produce a nonfluent or Wernicke’s aphasia- is spread more diffusely across the temporal and parietal lobes.[46]

Some areas of the brain are thought to be dedicated to handwriting. The dominant angular gyrus is believed to be involved in the abstract conversion of verbal representation of language to visual representation. While hypothesized by some authors to be involved in reading ability, other authors consider this area to be dedicated to written language production and instead implicate the nearby lateral occipital gyri in reading disorders.[13] Conversely, there is an area within the dominant middle frontal gyrus that has been referred to as the “graphemic/motor frontal area” (named in reference to a proposed “Exner’s area” of handwriting hypothesized by Siegmund Exner), which has been implicated as an interface between the abstract representation of words and the motor programming of writing.[47] Based on the above localization, it is plausible that lesions in the dominant angular gyrus may produce pure linguistic agraphia, and lesions to the middle frontal gyrus may produce pure apraxic agraphia.

Cases of lexical agraphia and phonological agraphia point to the existence of separate routes for lexical and phonological centers used for writing.[19] Of note, the graphemes of the English language--the letters of the Latin alphabet--are phonologic in nature, as they represent sounds. Research in agraphia in the Japanese language, which uses both morphologic and phonologic alphabets, gives support to the idea that there are separate morphologic and phonologic pathways for the production of reading and writing.[30][48][49]

While classic localization models remain clinically useful and aid in identifying cortical areas that are crucial in the various aspects of language, data increasingly support a network-based model of language processing. Functional imaging studies have revealed that language functions are more diffuse than previously thought.[50] Indeed, lesions to areas such as the cerebellum, thalamus, or even the nondominant hemisphere can mimic damage to Broca’s area or the posterior language area, which includes Wernicke’s area.[51][52] Similarly, clinical syndromes consistent with pure linguistic agraphia have been documented in the thalamus and internal capsule.[15][18]

In contrast to central agraphia, peripheral agraphia is much more varied in its pathophysiology and often localizes to one or more locations in the central and/or peripheral nervous system. The localization of apraxic agraphia is not standardized and has been most commonly documented with lesions in the frontal and parietal lobes, although lesions leading to apraxic agraphia have also been observed in the thalamus and cerebellum.[16][23] The possible role of Exner’s area in pure apraxic agraphia is postulated above. Of note, the agraphia that is part of the tetrad of Gerstmann syndrome, which classically implicates the posterior lobule of the dominant parietal lobe, has recently been challenged to represent separate apraxic agraphia involving the neighboring superior parietal gyrus.[53]

Paretic motor agraphia can occur due to lesions anywhere in the corticospinal tract and associated muscles. Micrographia is associated with Parkinsonism, which itself is most commonly associated with lesions to the substantia nigra, but can also occur with lesions in the globus pallidus, striatum, or even the frontal lobe.[54] Agraphia, due to tremors or chorea, can occur due to lesions in structures modulating motor control, including the basal ganglia or cerebellum.

Reiterative agraphia may reflect phenomena of conditions such as catatonia or Tourette syndrome.[55] When this feature is a reflection of perseverative behavior, this is thought to broadly localize to the frontal or parietal lobe.[56][57] Lesions anywhere in the optic pathway or areas of cortical visual processing can lead to visuospatial agraphia. Neglect, which classically localizes to the nondominant parietal lobe, can also lead to visuospatial agraphia. Functional agraphia is complex in nature and often does not easily localize to any particular structure.

History and Physical

Evaluating a patient’s writing ability is part of the complete neurologic workup for language, including fluency, comprehension, repetition, naming, and reading. The complete evaluation of both verbal and written language is essential to elucidating the cause of agraphia.

To assess writing in an English-speaking patient, the examiner should provide the patient with a writing instrument, a blank sheet of paper (or a writing board), and a flat writing surface. Patients lying in bed should be positioned upright. The examiner should then ask the patient to write a complete and grammatically correct sentence. This can be as simple as asking the patient to “Please write me a sentence,” but it can be helpful to offer open-ended prompts such as “What did you do today?”, “Why are you here?” or “Tell me something about yourself.” The examiner should encourage the patient to make their handwriting clear and legible.

Clinicians should observe the patient for correct orientation to the page and writing instrument in space. The act of writing should be observed for the general ease and speed of the task. The letters should be relatively uniform in size, spacing, and stroke quality. The examiner should note any errors in writing, including duplication of letter strokes, incorrect placement of spaces, or physical overlapping of words. Writing content should be analyzed for sentence length, word choice, accurate spelling, and correct use of grammatical elements and punctuation. Given the natural variation in individual handwriting, the examiner should compare pre-morbid writing samples if these are available or ask the patient if their handwriting appears different than it was previously.

The presence or absence of other neurological exam findings may help include or exclude different etiologies of agraphia. Patients with central agraphia often have concomitant aphasia. There may be a pertinent absence of paresis on manual muscle testing in patients with apraxic agraphia. Ideomotor apraxic agraphia may be accompanied by impaired use of other tools, and there can be deficits in mimicking tools such as a hammer or a toothbrush. Drawing an Archimedes spiral can help distinguish essential tremors from Parkinsonian tremors.[58] 

Micrographia can be accompanied by cogwheeling rigidity, decrements in fast finger tapping, or other features of Parkinsonism. An intact ability to copy a complex figure, such as the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test, may help exclude constructional apraxia when assessing apraxic agraphia.[28]

Evaluation

Bedside examination of agraphia by a clinician provides the initial characterization of writing dysfunction. In equivocal cases, a more extensive assessment can help further characterize agraphia. Specialized tests such as the Western Aphasia Battery or Boston Diagnostic Aphasia examination conducted by a speech and language pathologist can provide more granularity than a bedside clinical assessment and can aid in tracking the severity of agraphia.[59] These tests may also be administered by a neuropsychologist or psychometrician as part of an extensive neuropsychological evaluation.

Once agraphia is identified, localization should be clinically correlated to determine the underlying cause of the agraphia and guide further diagnostic workup. In some cases, as in the writer’s cramp, history and examination are sufficient to establish a diagnosis. Brain imaging with computerized tomography (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is often useful to localize or identify lesions causing agraphia, especially if caused by stroke. Volumetric MRI or neuropsychological testing may be helpful in cases of suspected dementia. Dopamine transporter single-photon emission computed tomography with an injection of ioflupane I-123 may aid in distinguishing idiopathic Parkinsonism from drug-induced Parkinsonism or essential tremor.[60]

Treatment / Management

The correct classification of a patient’s agraphia is paramount to proper treatment, as treatments vary greatly depending on the localization and etiology of the agraphia. In general, speech and language therapy (SLT) and occupational therapy are the cornerstones of treatment for both central and peripheral agraphia. Often, a multimodal approach including therapy, medication, and sometimes surgery is required to treat agraphia successfully: for example, a patient with micrographia due to Parkinson disease might require deep brain stimulation, antiparkinsonian medication, occupational therapy, and/or orthotic devices to optimally address their writing impairment.

In addition to addressing the symptoms of agraphia, it is essential to address the underlying etiology of agraphia to prevent the potential progression of writing impairment. For instance, a patient with a tumor leading to central agraphia should be offered appropriate chemotherapy, radiation, and/or surgical treatments.

Of the various etiologies of central agraphia, the strongest evidence is in central agraphia due to stroke. SLT is shown to be beneficial for language impairment after stroke, with evidence that high-dose SLT is favorable to low-dose SLT for improving writing ability.[5] One small study of 8 patients with alexia and agraphia due to stroke found benefit in targeted task-specific training in reading and/or writing, suggesting that treatment protocols may benefit from widely targeting a variety of specific writing deficits.[61]

Limited evidence suggests that piracetam may be useful in agraphia in the acute setting after a stroke.[62] Memantine has been shown to be effective in post-stroke aphasia but was not investigated for its efficacy on writing impairment.[63] Regarding neuromodulation, small trial data suggests a potential beneficial effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on writing impairment after stroke, although larger studies are needed.[64][65]

The treatment of peripheral agraphia is broader than central agraphia and depends on the subtype of agraphia. Visuospatial agraphia due to hemispatial neglect, for instance, may benefit from prism lenses.[66] The writer’s cramp is often amenable to local botulinum toxin injection.[32] Orthotic devices, occupational therapy, and relaxation techniques may also help with a writer’s cramp symptoms.[67]

Differential Diagnosis

Agraphia is not a single entity but a neurologic symptom with a multitude of etiologies. The “differential” for agraphia includes illiteracy, in which the ability to write is not acquired. Agraphia should also be distinguished from abulia, representing a generalized hypofunction and motivational deficit.

Prognosis

The prognosis of agraphia is highly variable and depends on its etiology. Broadly speaking, language impairment after a stroke follows a period of recovery that peaks around three months after a stroke, followed by an eventual plateau in language ability.[2][68] In contrast, due to neurodegenerative diseases, central agraphia is expected to worsen progressively. Peripheral agraphias comprise a wide variety of etiologies that carry different prognoses.

Complications

Complications of agraphia include issues with community integration, where functional communication is vital to independent daily life. Language impairments can be frustrating for patients, and in stroke, they are also associated with depression.[69] As agraphia is not a specific disease, the complications of the underlying etiology should be considered.

Deterrence and Patient Education

Patients and families should be educated about the presence and features of acquired language impairment. In otherwise cognitively intact patients, isolated language impairment can often be mistaken for global cognitive impairment. Patients should be provided with communication strategies individualized to their impairment. Even in the presence of reading language deficits, it can be helpful to periodically provide written educational material to patients with language impairment.[70]

Pearls and Other Issues

As with the various classifications for aphasias, there are many classification schemas to categorize agraphias. One such clinical neurological schema is used in this article. However, neuropsychological or psycholinguistic schemas to classify agraphia are also commonly used.[1][2]

Similarly, definitions and terminology in the study of neurologic language impairment can frequently be ambiguous or contradictory, with certain trends appearing in the literature over time. This is likely due partly to an understanding of agraphia that has evolved alongside technological advances.[71] Care is taken throughout this review to highlight relevant discrepancies and to minimize ambiguity.

In assessing agraphia, it is essential to consider a patient's education level. Illiteracy, in which written ability is never acquired, must be distinguished from agraphia. Literacy rates may vary depending on the practice setting. The patient's primary language should be considered, especially if it is different from the working language of the examiner. In-person or video-based professional language interpretation may be necessary for adequate assessment.

In our increasingly digitized world, the investigation of typing and texting--forms of non-motor writing--warrants consideration for inclusion in the standard clinical assessment of language.[9]

Enhancing Healthcare Team Outcomes

Language impairments can be frustrating for patients. When communicating with patients with language impairment in a healthcare setting, it is important to provide these individuals with ample time to express themselves and ensure that communication-related is clear and unambiguous. In a stroke, language impairments are associated with depression and poorer quality of life.[70] As such, the mental health of language-impaired patients should also be appropriately evaluated and addressed.

Good communication between interprofessional team members is vital to success when working in an interprofessional setting, as in rehabilitation. Agraphia and its features can be subtle, and relaying specific clinical findings directly to therapists can help guide effective speech and occupational therapy.

Rehabilitation for agraphia, as with other forms of disability, should be tailored to the patient’s individual capacity and goals. The need for writing as a skill varies between patients. As communication with language is inherently a social activity, it is also valuable, whenever possible, to engage the family members of patients with agraphia to help determine therapy goals.[72]


Details

Editor:

Alex R. Carter

Updated:

12/4/2022 2:47:05 PM

References


[1]

Roeltgen DP,Lacey EH, Reading, writing, and their disorders. Continuum (Minneapolis, Minn.). 2010 Aug;     [PubMed PMID: 22810513]


[2]

Sinanović O,Mrkonjić Z,Zukić S,Vidović M,Imamović K, Post-stroke language disorders. Acta clinica Croatica. 2011 Mar;     [PubMed PMID: 22034787]


[3]

Vandenborre D,van Dun K,Mariën P, Apraxic agraphia following bithalamic damage. Brain and cognition. 2015 Apr;     [PubMed PMID: 25682350]


[4]

Berthier ML, Poststroke aphasia : epidemiology, pathophysiology and treatment. Drugs     [PubMed PMID: 15733022]


[5]

Brady MC,Kelly H,Godwin J,Enderby P,Campbell P, Speech and language therapy for aphasia following stroke. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews. 2016 Jun 1;     [PubMed PMID: 27245310]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[6]

Basso A,Taborelli A,Vignolo LA, Dissociated disorders of speaking and writing in aphasia. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 1978 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 671067]


[7]

Assal G,Buttet J,Jolivet R, Dissociations in aphasia: a case report. Brain and language. 1981 Jul;     [PubMed PMID: 7260572]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[8]

Yoon JH,Suh MK,Kim H, Language-specific dysgraphia in Korean stroke patients. Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. 2010 Dec;     [PubMed PMID: 21150348]


[9]

Sharma AK,Fridman S,Gleichgerrcht E,Sposato LA, Dystextia and dystypia as modern stroke symptoms: A case series and literature review. Clinical neurology and neurosurgery. 2019 May;     [PubMed PMID: 30875514]

Level 2 (mid-level) evidence

[10]

Ellis AW,Miller D,Sin G, Wernicke's aphasia and normal language processing: a case study in cognitive neuropsychology. Cognition. 1983 Dec;     [PubMed PMID: 6686505]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[11]

Bub D,Kertesz A, Deep agraphia. Brain and language. 1982 Sep;     [PubMed PMID: 7139266]


[12]

Balasubramanian V, Dysgraphia in two forms of conduction aphasia. Brain and cognition. 2005 Feb;     [PubMed PMID: 15629207]


[13]

de Gobbi Porto FH,d'Ávila Freitas MI,de Oliveira MO,Lucato LT,Orsini M,de Menezes SL,Magaldi RM,Porto CS,Dozzi Brucki SM,Nitrini R, Thalamic alexia with agraphia. Neurology international. 2012 Jan 9;     [PubMed PMID: 22593808]


[14]

Yaguchi H,Yaguchi M,Bando M, [A case of pure agraphia due to left parietal lobe infarction]. No to shinkei = Brain and nerve. 2006 Oct;     [PubMed PMID: 17087281]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[15]

Toyokura M,Kobayashi R,Aono K, A case of pure agraphia due to left thalamic hemorrhage. The Tokai journal of experimental and clinical medicine. 2010 Sep 20;     [PubMed PMID: 21319033]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[16]

Krishnan G,Rao SN,Rajashekar B, Apraxic agraphia: An insight into the writing disturbances of posterior aphasias. Annals of Indian Academy of Neurology. 2009 Apr;     [PubMed PMID: 20142859]


[17]

Croisile B,Laurent B,Michel D,Trillet M, Pure agraphia after deep left hemisphere haematoma. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 1990 Mar;     [PubMed PMID: 2324759]


[18]

Laine T,Marttila RJ, Pure agraphia: a case study. Neuropsychologia. 1981;     [PubMed PMID: 7254510]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[19]

Roeltgen DP,Heilman KM, Lexical agraphia. Further support for the two-system hypothesis of linguistic agraphia. Brain : a journal of neurology. 1984 Sep;     [PubMed PMID: 6206909]


[20]

Gvion A,Friedmann N, Dyscravia: voicing substitution dysgraphia. Neuropsychologia. 2010 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 20298704]


[21]

Crary MA,Heilman KM, Letter imagery deficits in a case of pure apraxic agraphia. Brain and language. 1988 May;     [PubMed PMID: 3382929]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[22]

Otsuki M,Soma Y,Arai T,Otsuka A,Tsuji S, Pure apraxic agraphia with abnormal writing stroke sequences: report of a Japanese patient with a left superior parietal haemorrhage. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 1999 Feb;     [PubMed PMID: 10071107]


[23]

De Smet HJ,Engelborghs S,Paquier PF,De Deyn PP,Mariën P, Cerebellar-induced apraxic agraphia: a review and three new cases. Brain and cognition. 2011 Aug;     [PubMed PMID: 21507544]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[24]

Assmus A,Buss A,Milkereit EL,Meyer J,Fink GR, Pure apraxic agraphia: a disconnection syndrome after left subcortical stroke. European journal of neurology. 2007 Jan;     [PubMed PMID: 17222088]


[25]

Miyakawa Y,Fuchigami T,Aoki M,Mine Y,Suzuki J,Urakami T,Takahashi S, Agraphia with reversible splenial corpus callosum lesion caused by hypoglycemia. Brain     [PubMed PMID: 29606344]


[26]

Heilman KM, Apraxia. Continuum (Minneapolis, Minn.). 2010 Aug;     [PubMed PMID: 22810515]


[27]

Matiello M,Zimmerman E,Caplan D,Cohen AB, Reversible cursive agraphia. Neurology. 2015 Jul 21;     [PubMed PMID: 26195237]


[28]

Menichelli A,Rapp B,Semenza C, Allographic agraphia: a case study. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior. 2008 Jul-Aug;     [PubMed PMID: 18489965]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[29]

Rodrigues JC,da Fontoura DR,de Salles JF, Acquired dysgraphia in adults following right or left-hemisphere stroke. Dementia     [PubMed PMID: 29213909]


[30]

Pattamadilok C,Bulnes LC,Devlin JT,Bourguignon M,Morais J,Goldman S,Kolinsky R, How Early Does the Brain Distinguish between Regular Words, Irregular Words, and Pseudowords during the Reading Process? Evidence from Neurochronometric TMS. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2015 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 25603024]


[31]

Baxter DM,Warrington EK, Ideational agraphia: a single case study. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 1986 Apr;     [PubMed PMID: 3701346]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[32]

Kruisdijk JJ,Koelman JH,Ongerboer de Visser BW,de Haan RJ,Speelman JD, Botulinum toxin for writer's cramp: a randomised, placebo-controlled trial and 1-year follow-up. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 2007 Mar;     [PubMed PMID: 17185301]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[33]

Ardila A,Rosselli M, Spatial agraphia. Brain and cognition. 1993 Jul;     [PubMed PMID: 8373568]


[34]

Coslett HB, Apraxia, Neglect, and Agnosia. Continuum (Minneapolis, Minn.). 2018 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 29851877]


[35]

Auerbach SH,Alexander MP, Pure agraphia and unilateral optic ataxia associated with a left superior parietal lobule lesion. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 1981 May;     [PubMed PMID: 7264690]


[36]

Fridriksson J,den Ouden DB,Hillis AE,Hickok G,Rorden C,Basilakos A,Yourganov G,Bonilha L, Anatomy of aphasia revisited. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2018 Mar 1;     [PubMed PMID: 29360947]


[37]

Iscan D,Tolay R,Bayram E,Demir T,Bicakci S, Capecitabine-related neurotoxicity presenting with agraphia. Journal of oncology pharmacy practice : official publication of the International Society of Oncology Pharmacy Practitioners. 2022 Jul 29     [PubMed PMID: 35903929]


[38]

Salazar-Orellana JLI,Prado-Miranda G,Maldonado-Ortiz A, Agraphia: Presenting Feature of Syndrome of Transient Headache and Neurological Deficits With Cerebrospinal Fluid Lymphocytosis (HaNDL). Cureus. 2021 Feb 6     [PubMed PMID: 33717723]


[39]

Priftis K,Algeri L,Villella S,Spada MS, COVID-19 presenting with agraphia and conduction aphasia in a patient with left-hemisphere ischemic stroke. Neurological sciences : official journal of the Italian Neurological Society and of the Italian Society of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2020 Dec     [PubMed PMID: 32989587]


[40]

Utianski RL,Duffy JR,Savica R,Whitwell JL,Machulda MM,Josephs KA, Molecular neuroimaging in primary progressive aphasia with predominant agraphia. Neurocase. 2018 Apr;     [PubMed PMID: 29569990]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[41]

Gorno-Tempini ML,Hillis AE,Weintraub S,Kertesz A,Mendez M,Cappa SF,Ogar JM,Rohrer JD,Black S,Boeve BF,Manes F,Dronkers NF,Vandenberghe R,Rascovsky K,Patterson K,Miller BL,Knopman DS,Hodges JR,Mesulam MM,Grossman M, Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011 Mar 15;     [PubMed PMID: 21325651]


[42]

Engelter ST,Gostynski M,Papa S,Frei M,Born C,Ajdacic-Gross V,Gutzwiller F,Lyrer PA, Epidemiology of aphasia attributable to first ischemic stroke: incidence, severity, fluency, etiology, and thrombolysis. Stroke. 2006 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 16690899]


[43]

de Oliveira FF,Correia Marin Sde M,Ferreira Bertolucci PH, Communicating with the non-dominant hemisphere: Implications for neurological rehabilitation. Neural regeneration research. 2013 May 5;     [PubMed PMID: 25206418]


[44]

Skipper JI,Goldin-Meadow S,Nusbaum HC,Small SL, Speech-associated gestures, Broca's area, and the human mirror system. Brain and language. 2007 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 17533001]


[45]

Bernal B,Ardila A,Rosselli M, Broca's area network in language function: a pooling-data connectivity study. Frontiers in psychology. 2015;     [PubMed PMID: 26074842]


[46]

Binder JR, The Wernicke area: Modern evidence and a reinterpretation. Neurology. 2015 Dec 15;     [PubMed PMID: 26567270]


[47]

Roux FE,Dufor O,Giussani C,Wamain Y,Draper L,Longcamp M,Démonet JF, The graphemic/motor frontal area Exner's area revisited. Annals of neurology. 2009 Oct;     [PubMed PMID: 19847902]


[48]

Sakurai Y,Matsumura K,Iwatsubo T,Momose T, Frontal pure agraphia for kanji or kana: dissociation between morphology and phonology. Neurology. 1997 Oct;     [PubMed PMID: 9339672]


[49]

Sakurai Y, Kanji (Morphogram) and Kana (Phonogram) Problem in Japanese Alexia and Agraphia. Frontiers of neurology and neuroscience. 2019;     [PubMed PMID: 31220841]


[50]

Ferstl EC,Neumann J,Bogler C,von Cramon DY, The extended language network: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human brain mapping. 2008 May;     [PubMed PMID: 17557297]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[51]

Thiel A,Zumbansen A, The pathophysiology of post-stroke aphasia: A network approach. Restorative neurology and neuroscience. 2016 Jun 13;     [PubMed PMID: 27314978]


[52]

Mariën P,Saerens J,Nanhoe R,Moens E,Nagels G,Pickut BA,Dierckx RA,De Deyn PP, Cerebellar induced aphasia: case report of cerebellar induced prefrontal aphasic language phenomena supported by SPECT findings. Journal of the neurological sciences. 1996 Dec;     [PubMed PMID: 8994102]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[53]

Ardila A, A proposed reinterpretation of Gerstmann's syndrome. Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 2014 Dec;     [PubMed PMID: 25377466]


[54]

Joutsa J,Horn A,Hsu J,Fox MD, Localizing parkinsonism based on focal brain lesions. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2018 Aug 1;     [PubMed PMID: 29982424]


[55]

Cavanna AE,Ali F,Rickards H, Paligraphia and written jocularity in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 2011 Apr;     [PubMed PMID: 21437984]


[56]

Reber J,Tranel D, Frontal lobe syndromes. Handbook of clinical neurology. 2019;     [PubMed PMID: 31590727]


[57]

Kleinman JT,DuBois JC,Newhart M,Hillis AE, Disentangling the neuroanatomical correlates of perseveration from unilateral spatial neglect. Behavioural neurology. 2013;     [PubMed PMID: 22713393]


[58]

Alty J,Cosgrove J,Thorpe D,Kempster P, How to use pen and paper tasks to aid tremor diagnosis in the clinic. Practical neurology. 2017 Dec;     [PubMed PMID: 28844041]


[59]

Gilmore N,Dwyer M,Kiran S, Benchmarks of Significant Change After Aphasia Rehabilitation. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2019 Jun;     [PubMed PMID: 30240594]


[60]

Pagano G,Niccolini F,Politis M, Imaging in Parkinson's disease. Clinical medicine (London, England). 2016 Aug;     [PubMed PMID: 27481384]


[61]

Johnson JP, Ross K, Kiran S. Multi-step treatment for acquired alexia and agraphia (Part I): efficacy, generalisation, and identification of beneficial treatment steps. Neuropsychological rehabilitation. 2019 May:29(4):534-564. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2017.1311271. Epub 2017 Apr 19     [PubMed PMID: 28421858]


[62]

Greener J,Enderby P,Whurr R, Pharmacological treatment for aphasia following stroke. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews. 2001;     [PubMed PMID: 11687079]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[63]

Berthier ML,Green C,Lara JP,Higueras C,Barbancho MA,Dávila G,Pulvermüller F, Memantine and constraint-induced aphasia therapy in chronic poststroke aphasia. Annals of neurology. 2009 May;     [PubMed PMID: 19475666]


[64]

Ren CL,Zhang GF,Xia N,Jin CH,Zhang XH,Hao JF,Guan HB,Tang H,Li JA,Cai DL, Effect of low-frequency rTMS on aphasia in stroke patients: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PloS one. 2014;     [PubMed PMID: 25036386]

Level 1 (high-level) evidence

[65]

Ramos NCDC,Pietrobon CA,Rocha RMG,Martini LLL,Buratto LG,Cera ML, Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in writing: a case report of deep agraphia. CoDAS. 2022;     [PubMed PMID: 35137891]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[66]

Rusconi ML,Carelli L, Long-term efficacy of prism adaptation on spatial neglect: preliminary results on different spatial components. TheScientificWorldJournal. 2012;     [PubMed PMID: 22629167]


[67]

Singam NV,Dwivedi A,Espay AJ, Writing Orthotic Device for the Management of Writer's Cramp. Frontiers in neurology. 2013;     [PubMed PMID: 23372563]


[68]

Hamilton RH,Chrysikou EG,Coslett B, Mechanisms of aphasia recovery after stroke and the role of noninvasive brain stimulation. Brain and language. 2011 Jul;     [PubMed PMID: 21459427]


[69]

Spaccavento S,Craca A,Del Prete M,Falcone R,Colucci A,Di Palma A,Loverre A, Quality of life measurement and outcome in aphasia. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment. 2014;     [PubMed PMID: 24368886]

Level 2 (mid-level) evidence

[70]

Rose T,Worrall L,Hickson L,Hoffmann T, Do people with aphasia want written stroke and aphasia information? A verbal survey exploring preferences for when and how to provide stroke and aphasia information. Topics in stroke rehabilitation. 2010 Mar-Apr;     [PubMed PMID: 20542851]

Level 3 (low-level) evidence

[71]

Henderson VW, Chapter 37: alexia and agraphia. Handbook of clinical neurology. 2010;     [PubMed PMID: 19892140]


[72]

Howe T,Davidson B,Worrall L,Hersh D,Ferguson A,Sherratt S,Gilbert J, 'You needed to rehab … families as well': family members' own goals for aphasia rehabilitation. International journal of language     [PubMed PMID: 22938062]